
DOES ALL WINE TASTE THE SAME? YOU BE THE JUDGE!

I recently posted an article entitled The “Judgment of Princeton” (to read that article click here). This was an account of a recent blind tasting of New Jersey wines vs. French wines. A friend also sent me a recent article featuring the Judgment of Princeton. It was from a June 13, 2012 posting on The New Yorker magazine blog written by Jonah Lehrer and entitled “Does All Wine Taste the Same?” (to read that article click here).
Here are some highlights from that article:
- “…The Judgment of Princeton didn’t quite end with a Jersey victory—a French wine was on top in both the red and white categories—but, in terms of the reassurance for those with valuable wine collections, it might as well have. Clos des Mouches only narrowly beat out Unionville Single Vineyard and two other Jersey whites, while Château Mouton Rothschild and Haut-Brion topped Heritage’s BDX. The wines from New Jersey cost, on average, about five per cent as much as their French counterparts. And then there’s the inconsistency of the judges: the scores for that Mouton Rothschild, for instance, ranged from 11 to 19.5….
- …What can we learn from these tests? First, that tasting wine is really hard, even for experts. Because the sensory differences between different bottles of rotten grape juice are so slight—and the differences get even more muddled after a few sips—there is often wide disagreement about which wines are best. For instance, both the winning red and white wines in the Princeton tasting were ranked by at least one of the judges as the worst.
- The perceptual ambiguity of wine helps explain why contextual influences—say, the look of a label, or the price tag on the bottle—can profoundly influence expert judgment. This was nicely demonstrated in a mischievous 2001 experiment led by Frédéric Brochet at the University of Bordeaux. In the first test, Brochet invited fifty-seven wine experts and asked them to give their impressions of what looked like two glasses of red and white wine. The wines were actually the same white wine, one of which had been tinted red with food coloring. But that didn’t stop the experts from describing the “red” wine in language typically used to describe red wines. One expert praised its “jamminess,” while another enjoyed its “crushed red fruit.
- The second test Brochet conducted was even more damning. He took a middling Bordeaux and served it in two different bottles. One bottle bore the label of a fancy grand cru, the other of an ordinary vin de table. Although they were being served the exact same wine, the experts gave the bottles nearly opposite descriptions. The grand cru was summarized as being “agreeable,” “woody,” “complex,” “balanced,” and “rounded,” while the most popular adjectives for the vin de table included “weak,” “short,” “light,” “flat,” and “faulty.”
- The results are even more distressing for non-experts. In recent decades, the wine world has become an increasingly quantitative place, as dependent on scores and statistics as Billy Beane. But these ratings suggest a false sense of precision, as if it were possible to reliably identify the difference between an eighty-nine-point Merlot from Jersey and a ninety-one-point blend from Bordeaux—or even a greater spread. And so we linger amid the wine racks, paralyzed by the alcoholic arithmetic. How much are we willing to pay for a few extra points?
- These calculations are almost certainly a waste of time. Last year, the psychologist Richard Wiseman bought a wide variety of bottles at the local supermarket, from a five-dollar Bordeaux to a fifty-dollar champagne, and asked people to say which wine was more expensive. (All of the taste tests were conducted double-blind, with neither the experimenter nor subject aware of the actual price.) According to Wiseman’s data, the five hundred and seventy-eight participants could only pick the more expensive wine fifty-three per cent of the time, which is basically random chance. They actually performed below chance when it came to picking red wines. Bordeaux fared the worst, with a significant majority—sixty-one per cent—picking the cheap plonk as the more expensive selection.”
- Regarding the results of the Judgment of Princeton: “These results raise an obvious question: if most people can’t tell the difference between Château Mouton Rothschild (retail: seven hundred and twenty-five dollars) and Heritage BDX (thirty-five dollars from the winery), then why do we splurge on premiers crus? Why not drink Jersey grapes instead? It seems like a clear waste of money.
- The answer returns us to the sensory limitations of the mind. If these blind testings teach us anything, it’s that for the vast majority of experts and amateurs fine-grained perceptual judgments are impossible. Instead, as Brochet points out, our expectations of the wine are often more important than what’s actually in the glass. When we take a sip of wine, we don’t taste the wine first, and the cheapness or expensiveness second. We taste everything all at once, in a single gulp of thiswineisMoutonRothschild, or thiswineisfromSouthJersey. As a result, if we think a wine is cheap, then it will taste cheap. And if we think we are tasting a premier cru, then we will taste a premier cru. Our senses are vague in their instructions, and we parse their inputs based upon whatever other knowledge we can summon to the surface. It’s not that those new French oak barrels or carefully pruned vines don’t matter—it’s that the logo on the bottle and price tag often matter more….”
My friend who sent the article also commented that all such reports about tasting wine fail to mention one crucial element: memory. I completely agree. In order to perceive how a wine should taste you need to be able to remember how it has tasted in the past. If you have never tasted the wine before or if you can’t remember, then you at a further disadvantage to try to guess what it is, but not to know if you like it or not.
The Bottom Line: People’s ability to taste varies tremendously. The perception of taste is also influenced by what you see and what you know or what think you know which includes your memory and recall. That’s why a number does not mean much. It is one subjective opinion based on many factors which may or may not be based on the ability to taste, knowledge, or recall. It is, in fact, that such opinions are likely to be many as there are tasters. The results of many studies and tastings like the ones mentioned here confirm these facts. So if you don’t agree with a number, don’t get your knickers in a twist. You really don’t need a number. Drink What You Like & Like What You Drink! (to see my article on this click here)
In Vino Veritas,
John Tilson
Article Tags:
ambiguity,
brochet,
château mouton rothschild,
contextual influences,
expert judgment,
first test,
french counterparts,
french wine,
french wines,
grape juice,
Haut Brion,
how to taste wine,
inconsistency,
jersey wines,
Judgment of Princeton,
Mouton Rothschild,
New Jersey wines,
new yorker magazine,
perception of taste,
reassurance,
recent article,
sips,
tasting wine,
white wines,
wine collections,
wine taste
No comments so far for “DON’T GET YOUR KNICKERS IN A TWIST!”